Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorZawiszewski, Adam ORCID
dc.contributor.authorMartínez de la Hidalga Malla, Gillen ORCID
dc.contributor.authorLaka Mugarza, Itziar ORCID
dc.date.accessioned2022-08-05T12:11:25Z
dc.date.available2022-08-05T12:11:25Z
dc.date.issued2022
dc.identifier.citationBrain Sciences 12(7) : (2022) // Article ID 853es_ES
dc.identifier.issn2076-3425
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10810/57232
dc.description.abstractAre non-native speakers able to process their second language in a native-like way? The present study used the Event-Related Potentials’ (ERPs) method to address this issue by focusing (1) on agent vs. agentless intransitive sentences and (2) on person vs. number agreement morphology. For that purpose, native and high proficiency and early non-native speakers of Spanish were tested while processing intransitive sentences containing grammatical and ungrammatical subject–verb agreement. Results reveal greater accuracy in the agent (unergative) condition as compared with the agentless (unaccusative) condition and different ERP patterns for both types of verbs in all participants, suggesting a larger processing cost for the agentless sentences than for the agentive ones. These effects were more pronounced in the native group as compared with the non-native one in the early time window (300–500 ms). Differences between person and number agreement processing were also found at both behavioral and electrophysiological levels, indicating that those morphological features are distinctively processed. Importantly, this pattern of results held for both native and non-native speakers, thus suggesting that native-like competence is attainable given early Age of Acquisition (AoA), frequent use and high proficiency.es_ES
dc.description.sponsorshipThis research was supported by grants from the Spanish Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación (FPI-2017-BES-2016-076456) (G.M.d.l.H.), Ministerio de Ciencia, Innovación y Universidades (PID2019-104016GB-I00), and the Basque Government (IT1169-19).es_ES
dc.language.isoenges_ES
dc.publisherMDPIes_ES
dc.relationinfo:eu-repo/grantAgreement/MINECO/FPI-2017-BES-2016-076456es_ES
dc.relationinfo:eu-repo/grantAgreement/MICIU/PID2019-104016GB-I00es_ES
dc.rightsinfo:eu-repo/semantics/openAccesses_ES
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
dc.subjectintransitive sentence processinges_ES
dc.subjectERPses_ES
dc.subjectagentses_ES
dc.subjectpatientses_ES
dc.subjectnon-native language processinges_ES
dc.titleAgents Strongly Preferred: ERP Evidence from Natives and Non-Natives Processing Intransitive Sentences in Spanishes_ES
dc.typeinfo:eu-repo/semantics/articlees_ES
dc.date.updated2022-07-25T16:32:41Z
dc.rights.holder© 2022 by the authors.Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https:// creativecommons.org/licenses/by/ 4.0/).es_ES
dc.relation.publisherversionhttps://www.mdpi.com/2076-3425/12/7/853es_ES
dc.identifier.doi10.3390/brainsci12070853
dc.departamentoesLingüística y estudios vascos
dc.departamentoeuHizkuntzalaritza eta euskal ikasketak


Files in this item

Thumbnail

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record

© 2022 by the authors.Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https:// creativecommons.org/licenses/by/ 4.0/).
Except where otherwise noted, this item's license is described as © 2022 by the authors.Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https:// creativecommons.org/licenses/by/ 4.0/).