Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorBolognesi, Marianna M.
dc.contributor.authorCombei, Claudia Roberta
dc.contributor.authorLa Pietra, Marta
dc.contributor.authorMasini, Francesa
dc.date.accessioned2024-05-21T14:11:37Z
dc.date.available2024-05-21T14:11:37Z
dc.date.issued2024
dc.identifier.citationBolognesi MM, Combei CR, La Pietra M, Masini F. What makes an awfully good oxymoron? Language and Cognition. 2024;16(1):242-262. doi:10.1017/langcog.2023.68es_ES
dc.identifier.citationLanguage and Cognition
dc.identifier.issn1065-9471
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10810/68063
dc.descriptionPublished onilne 18 January 2024es_ES
dc.description.abstractOxymorons combine two opposite terms in a paradoxical manner. They are closely intertwined with antonymy, since the union of antonymous items creates the paradoxical effect of the oxymoron and generates a new meaning. Compared to other forms of figurative language, oxymorons are largely underinvestigated. We explored what makes good oxymorons through a crowdsourcing task in which we asked participants to judge the acceptability, comprehensibility, effectiveness/aptness, commonness, pleasantness, and humoristic connotation of Italian adjective–noun oxymorons. We hypothesized that oxymorons featuring morphologically related antonyms (felice infelicità ‘happy unhappiness’) may be perceived to be better than oxymorons featuring morphologically unrelated antonyms (felice tristezza ‘happy sadness’) and that oxymorons constructed by complementaries (esatta inesattezza ‘exact inexactness’) may be perceived to be better than oxymorons constructed by contraries (bella bruttezza ‘beautiful ugliness’). The results confirmed only partially our hypotheses: oxymorons with complementaries were perceived as more acceptable, comprehensible, effective/apt, common, whereas no strong trend was found for the other two dimensions. Surprisingly, our analyses revealed that oxymoronic constructions containing morphologically unrelated words were perceived as more acceptable, comprehensible, effective/apt, common, pleasant, contradicting our initial expectations.es_ES
dc.description.sponsorshipC.R.C. is supported by the project ‘Pon Ricerca e Innovazione 2014–2020 (D.M. 1062/2021)’, while M.L.P. is supported by grant ref. PRE2021-097880; project PID2020-114717 funded by MCIN/AEI/10.13039/501100011033 and FSE+.es_ES
dc.language.isoenges_ES
dc.publisherELSEVIERes_ES
dc.relationinfo:eu-repo/grantAgreement/AEI/PRE2021-097880es_ES
dc.relationinfo:eu-repo/grantAgreement/AEI/PID2020-114717es_ES
dc.relationinfo:eu-repo/grantAgreement/MCIN/AEI/10.13039/501100011033es_ES
dc.rightsinfo:eu-repo/semantics/openAccesses_ES
dc.subjectantonymyes_ES
dc.subjectcrowdsourcinges_ES
dc.subjectfigurative languagees_ES
dc.subjectItalianes_ES
dc.subjectoxymorones_ES
dc.titleWhat makes an awfully good oxymoron?es_ES
dc.typeinfo:eu-repo/semantics/articlees_ES
dc.rights.holder© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press.This is an Open Access article,distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.es_ES
dc.relation.publisherversionhttps://www.cambridge.org/universitypresses_ES
dc.identifier.doi10.1002/hbm.26564


Files in this item

Thumbnail

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record